ⓘ Actor–observer asymmetry explains the errors that one makes when forming attributions about the behavior of others. When people judge their own behavior, and th ..

                                     

ⓘ Actor–observer asymmetry

Actor–observer asymmetry explains the errors that one makes when forming attributions about the behavior of others. When people judge their own behavior, and they are the actor, they are more likely to attribute their actions to the particular situation than to a generalization about their personality. Yet when an observer is explaining the behavior of another person, they are more likely to attribute this behavior to the actors overall disposition rather than to situational factors. This frequent error shows the bias that people hold in their evaluations of behavior. Because people are better acquainted with the situational factors affecting their own decisions, they are more likely to see their own behavior as affected by the social situation they are in. However, because the situational effects of anothers behavior are less accessible to the observer, observers see the actors behavior as influenced more by the actors overall personality. The actor-observer asymmetry is a component of the ultimate attribution error. Sometimes the Actor–observer asymmetry is defined as the fundamental attribution error which is when people tend to focus on the internal, personal characteristic or disposition as cause of a behavior rather than the external factors or situational influences. The actor-observer asymmetry tends to happen in events where people express behavioral emotion, such a first meeting, a blind date, a shopping at a supermarket etc. From a study by Sheldon and Johnson when asking people which object they have noticed when talking with another person, their common answers were based on their own thoughts and the other persons appearance.

This term falls under "attribution" or "attribution theory". The specific hypothesis of an actor-observer asymmetry in attribution explanations of behavior was originally proposed by Jones and Nisbett 1971, when they claimed that "actors tend to attribute the causes of their behavior to stimuli inherent in the situation, while observers tend to attribute behavior to stable dispositions of the actor” Jones & Nisbett 1971, p. 93. Supported by initial evidence, the hypothesis was long held as firmly established, describing a robust and pervasive phenomenon of social cognition.

However, a meta-analysis of all the published tests of the hypothesis between 1971 and 2004 Malle 2006 yielded a contradictory finding: there was no actor-observer asymmetry of the sort Jones & Nisbett 1971 had proposed. Malle 2006 interpreted this result not so much as proof that actors and observers explained behavior exactly the same way but as evidence that the original hypothesis was fundamentally flawed in the way it framed peoples explanations of behavior - namely, as attributions to either stable dispositions or to the situation. Against the background of a different theory of explanation, Malle et al. 2007 tested an alternative set of three actor-observer asymmetries and found consistent support for all of them. Thus, the actor-observer asymmetry does not exist in one theoretical formulation traditional attribution theory but does exist in the new alternative theoretical formulation. Malle 2011 argued that this favors the alternative theoretical formulation, but, at the time of writing, textbooks had not yet fully addressed this theoretical challenge.

Considerations of actor-observer differences can be found in other disciplines as well, such as philosophy e.g. privileged access, incorrigibility, management studies, artificial intelligence, semiotics, anthropology, and political science.

                                     

1. Background and initial formulation

The background of this hypothesis was in the 1960s, with social psychologys increasing interest in the cognitive mechanisms by which people make sense of their own and other peoples behavior. This interest was instigated by Fritz Heiders 1958 book, The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations, and the research in its wake has become known as "attribution research" or "attribution theory."

The specific hypothesis of an "actor–observer asymmetry" was first proposed by social psychologists Jones and Nisbett in 1971. Jones and Nisbett hypothesized that these two roles actors and observers produce asymmetric explanations. Their research findings showed that" there is pervasive tendency for actors to attribute their actions to situational requirements, whereas observers tend to attribute the same actions to stable personal dispositions”. For example, a student who studies hard for an exam is likely to explain her own the actors intensive studying by referring to the upcoming difficult exam a situational factor, whereas other people the observers are likely to explain her studying by referring to her dispositions, such as being hardworking or ambitious.

                                     

2. Early evidence and reception

Soon after the publication of the actor-observer hypothesis, numerous research studies tested its validity, most notably the first such test by Nisbett et al. 1973. The authors found initial evidence for the hypothesis, and so did Storms 1973, who also examined one possible explanation of the hypothesis: that actors explain their behaviors by reference to the situation because they attend to the situation not to their own behaviors whereas observers explain the actors behavior by reference to the actors dispositions because they attend to the actors behavior not to the situation. Based largely on this initial supporting evidence, the confidence in the hypothesis became uniformly high. The asymmetry was described as" robust and quite general”, "firmly established" and" an entrenched part of scientific psychology”. Likewise, evidence for the asymmetry was considered to be "plentiful” and" pervasive”.

                                     

3. Recent evidence

Over 100 studies have been published since 1971 in which the hypothesis was put to further tests often in the context of testing another hypothesis about causal attributions. Malle 2006 examined this entire literature in a meta-analysis, which is a robust way of identifying consistent patterns of evidence regarding a given hypothesis across a broad set of studies. The result of this analysis was stunning: across 170 individual tests, the asymmetry practically did not exist. Under circumscribed conditionsi.e. if the actor was portrayed as highly idiosyncratic, or in negative events, it could sometimes be found, but under other conditions, the opposite was found. The conclusion was that the widely held assumption of an actor-observer asymmetry in attribution was false.

In contrast to the Malle 2006 article, other research has shown a strong presence of the actor-observer asymmetry even in instances with familiar people. Krueger et al. 1996 conducted a study on pairs of university dorm roommates who liked and knew one another well. The researchers aimed for familiar pairs of participants was to discover whether or not actor-observer asymmetry existed in conditions that might atypically work against it. Previous literature suggests that actor-observer asymmetry would not be present in situations where the actors and the observers were familiar with each other, which is why Krueger and colleagues wanted to perform the study with familiar pairs. Each participant answered three questionnaires where the final scores were weighed against each other in order to understand the presence of actor-observer asymmetry. The results showed that gender did not affect the findings, so whether or not the pairs were the same or opposite sex was not a mediator for the data. The researchers found that actors were aware of the actor-observer asymmetry, but the observers were not, which is typically what happens in everyday life. Krueger and colleagues showed another side to the actor-observer asymmetry, wherein it is present even among familiar people.

Even more recent evidence was published on the social acceptability of actions and the speed with which an observers perception of an actors moral character are determined and affected by actor-observer asymmetry. Critcher et al. 2012 conducted two experiments in order to support the idea that an immoral action is quickly followed by a negative evaluation of the actors moral character by the observer. On the other hand, a morally good decision by an actor is readily given a positive evaluation of that actors moral character. This is due to the fact that actions are observed as having been made with a degree of certainty and intentionality on the part of the actor, and more distinct motives are the underlying cause of these actions, thus creating more contrasted evaluations of the actor by the observer.

Certain emotions have also been shown to affect actor-observer bias. In their 2013 study called "Emotion and the Ultimate Attribution Error", researcher Martin D. Coleman took 420 participants and asked them how they attributed the misbehavior of specific politicians, Republicans and Democrats respectively, to see if their actor-observer bias was affected by emotion. The participants were asked whether the politicians misdeed made them angry, fearful, or feel no emotion in particular. the studys results concluded that when feeling angry or in fear, the participates more easily used actor-observer asymmetry to judge the actions of either a Democratic or Republican politician. Coleman also found that in-group/ favoring bias combined with emotion made the participants more susceptible to using actor-observer bias.The findings in this study have been corroborated by further analysis via psychology professor D.J. Northingtons 2015 study, "An Attribution-Emotion Approach to Political Conflict", in which political leanings are shown to create in-group bias that influences emotions toward out-groupers, and further influences actor-observer asymmetry. 564 participants were polled in this study, asked for their political affiliation, and read headlines about either a Democratic or Republican candidates misdeeds. Again, the emotions elicited, namely anger, allowed actor-observer asymmetry to affect the judgement of the participants.



                                     

4. Theoretical reformulation

The result of the meta-analysis implied that, across the board, actors and observers explain behaviors the same way. But all the tests of the classic hypothesis presupposed that people explain behavior by referring to "dispositional" vs. "situational" causes. This assumption turned out to be incorrect for the class of behavioral events that people explain most frequently in real life Malle & Knobe 1997: intentional behaviors. People explain unintentional behaviors in ways that the traditional disposition-situation framework can capture, but they explain intentional behaviors by using very different concepts Buss, 1978; Heider 1958. A recent empirical theory of how people explain behavior was proposed and tested by Malle 1999, 2004, centering on the postulate that intentional behaviors are typically explained by reasons - the mental states typically beliefs and desires in light of which and on the grounds of which the agent decided to act a postulate long discussed in the philosophy of action. But people who explain intentional behavior have several choices to make, and the theory identifies the psychological antecedents and consequences of these choices:

Empirical studies have so far supported this theoretical framework.

Within this framework, the actor-observer asymmetry was then reformulated as in fact consisting of three asymmetries: that actors offer more reason explanations relative to CHR explanations than observers do; that actors offer more belief reasons relative to desire reasons than observers do; and that actors use fewer belief reason markers than observers do Malle 1999. Malle et al. 2007 tested these asymmetries across 9 studies and found consistent support for them. In the same studies they also tested the classic person/disposition vs. situation hypothesis and consistently found no support for it.

Thus, people do seem to explain their own actions differently from how they explain other peoples actions. But these differences do not lie in a predominance of using "dispositional" vs. "situational" causes. Only when peoples explanations are separated into theoretically meaningful distinctions e.g., reasons vs. causal history of reason explanations do the differences emerge.

In addition, an alternative theory has been proposed called the folk-conceptual theory. In contrast to the actor-observer asymmetry, it posits that peoples explanations of behavior varies based on three key parameters.

                                     

4.1. Theoretical reformulation Implications

The choices of different explanations for intentional behavior indicate particular psychological functions. Reasons, for example, appear to reflect among other things psychological closeness. People increase reason explanations relative to CHR explanations when they explain their own rather than another persons behavior Malle et al. 2007, when they portray another person in a positive light Malle et al. 2007, and when they explain behaviors of nonhuman agents for whom they have ownership and affection. Conversely, people use fewer reasons and more CHR explanations when explaining behaviors of collectives or aggregate groups OLaughlin & Malle 2002. Actor-observer asymmetries can therefore be seen as part of a broader continuum of psychological distance people have to various kinds of minds.

                                     

5. Cultural differences

Cultural differences may impact how certain behaviors or actions are attributed and interpreted. Current research supports the idea that Western culture emphasizes individualism, whereas East Asian cultures emphasize collectivism. The fundamental attribution error differs in those cultures. In the Individualistic cultures people tend to favor dispositional explanations for behavior. Whereas, in the collectivist cultures where P. B. Smith and Bond 1994 implied that the fundamental attribution error is minimal or even absent, so they tend to focus on situational explanation for behavior. Masuda & Nisbett 2001 found when viewing an underwater scene Americans focused more on fish in the foreground and the direction they were swimming within the tank than the background of the environment. This supports the idea that Americans are more like to attribute behavior to dispositional cue that are directly present in the environment or foreground. This is opposed to Japanese participants who focused on the fish, but additionally focused on the background of the environment plants, other animals. This shows how people from East Asian cultures are more likely to attribute behavior to both dispositional and situational cues in the environment. In addition, Choi & Nisbett 2008 found that when situational constraints of participants in an experiment were made more salient that only the East Asian participants had an increased perception of the situational constraints and made their judgments accordingly. This is opposed to North American participants who showed little to no change in perception of the situational constraints as they were made more salient.

Recent studies have examined the impact culture has on actor-observer asymmetry. Researchers Thomas D. Green and Duane G. McClearn in their 2010 study, "The actor- observer effect as a function of performance outcome and nationality of other", took a group of 55 American college students from an unspecified southeastern institute in the United States and walked them through a list of hypothetical scenarios to determine if a persons nationality effected how the students viewed, or observed,the scenarios outcome. Each scenario contained an actor of following nationality; Mexican, Japanese, Russian, English and American, in which the student and the actor would take part in a task that relied on collaboration. After completion, the students were asked to grade the outcomes as successful A in examination or unsuccessful F in examination and the performance of the actor and themselves. The studys results showed that the students consistently rated their own performances highly, in keeping with the hypothesis that individuals will attribute errors to external factors,and the performance of the actors low. Results began to vary, however, when determining success. the scenarios that where rated unsuccessful involved the Japanese and Russian actors consistently, while the successful scenarios involved the Canadian Mexican, English and American actors, showing a correlation between proximity of neighboring countries and actor-observer asymmetry.

Another study published in 2019 in Social Psychological and Personality Science by researchers Anita Korner, Sophie Moritz, and Roland Deutsch showed how distance, in relation to space or mentality, can further enable actor-observer asymmetry. Participants were tested on how they viewed the situation; unemployment, homelessness or poverty, of a person they were close to and a stranger. They consistently rated the situation of an unfamiliar person a stranger as having internal origins bad with money, lazy and the situation of a familiar person as having external origin. Actor-observer asymmetry is more commonly applied to those at a distance than those in proximity.

Additionally, it has differed in religious perspective. Protestant are most likely to focus on internal factor whether than external for behavior. Unlike the Catholic who would tend to focus on the external factor. The cause is that Protestant rely too much on correlational evidence without evidence of causality MacKinnon, 2008. It is also cause by the fact that Protestant have stronger faith and are more aware of the souls condition than Catholic.



                                     

6.1. Related but distinct concepts Actor-observer "bias"

Instead of speaking of a hypothesis of an actor-observer asymmetry, some textbooks and research articles speak of an "actor-observer bias." The term "bias" is typically used to imply that one of the explainers either the actor or the observer is biased or incorrect in their explanations. But which one - the actor or the observer - is supposed to be incorrect is not clear from the literature. On the one hand, Rosss 1977 hypothesis of a "fundamental attribution error" suggests that observers are incorrect, because they show a general tendency to overemphasize dispositional explanations and underemphasize situational ones. On the other hand, Nisbett and Wilson 1977 argued that actors dont really know the true causes of their actions the so-called "introspection illusion" and often merely invent plausible explanations. Jones & Nisbett 1971 themselves did not commit to calling the hypothesized actor-observer asymmetry a bias or an error. Similarly, recent theoretical positions consider asymmetries not a bias, but rather the result of multiple cognitive and motivational differences that fundamentally exist between actors and observers.



                                     

6.2. Related but distinct concepts Self-serving bias

The actor-observer asymmetry is often confused with the hypothesis of a self-serving bias in attribution - the claim that people choose explanations in a strategic way so as to make themselves appear in a more positive light. The important difference between the two hypotheses is that the assumed actor-observer asymmetry is expected to hold for all events and behaviors whether they are positive or negative and require a specific comparison between actor explanations and observer explanations. The self-serving bias is often formulated as a complete reversal in actors and observers explanation tendencies as a function of positive vs. negative events. In traditional attribution terms, this means that for positive events e.g., getting an A on an exam, actors will select explanations that refer to their own dispositions, e.g., "I am smart" whereas observers will select explanations that refer to the actors situation e.g., "The test was easy"; however, for negative events e.g., receiving an F on the exam, actors will select explanations that refer to the situation, e.g., "The test was impossibly hard" whereas observers will select explanations that refer to the actors dispositions e.g., "She is not smart enough".

                                     

6.3. Related but distinct concepts Positivity bias

The actor-observer asymmetry can seem similar to the hypothesis of a positivity bias in attribution- the claim that people are biased toward favorable evaluations. This hypothesis states that people will attribute their behavior with positive consequences to internal factors and their behavior with negative consequences to external factors. The positivity bias is described in terms of the actors attributions of their own behavior. This means that people will attribute their behavior which received a positive consequence passes their driving test and receiving their drivers license to an internal factor I really know the material. However, people will attribute their behavior in which they received a negative consequence failing a driving test to an external factor the sun was in my eyes.

                                     

6.4. Related but distinct concepts Correspondence bias

Observers attribute actions of others to their future behavior. Witnessing ones actions brings the witness to attribute those same actions to that persons future behavior. This explains why first impressions are so important to us. Once an action is seen, it is hard for the observer to imagine any other differing behaviors from the actor. However, on the other hand, it is hard for actors to attribute one action they have made to their whole behavior. They view themselves as more responsive, and therefore believe themselves to be in control of all situational matters. As the actor can attribute every action in the past he/she has done, the observer can only attribute the one action that is witnessed to that actor. Therefore, will attribute dispositional, rather than situational means to the actor.

                                     
  • The actor - observer bias also actor observer asymmetry can be thought of as an extension of the fundamental attribution error. According to the actor - observer
  • pervasive meta - analysis of the 173 qualified studies of the actor - observer asymmetry available by 2005 established, surprisingly, an effect size of
  • qualities lack of ability, laziness, ineptitude, etc. their actor - observer asymmetry argument forms the basis of discourse on trait ascription bias
  • In contrast, to Taleb, the problem is more fundamentally one of asymmetry one actor gets the rewards, the other is stuck with the risks. Taleb argues
  • outgroup - serving attributions for group differences. Psychology portal Actor observer asymmetry Cultural bias Dispositional attribution Fundamental attribution
  • participants as the actor of a task or as the observer of someone else performing a task, relating closely to actor observer asymmetry Actors of a task exhibit
  • which prevents total victory by one side and results in the creation of asymmetries between belligerents which opens the door for continued bloodshed. In
  • unfairly for one s own benefit. It is a social relationship based on an asymmetry in a power relationship between workers and their employers. When speaking
  • nonequity crowdfunding, equity crowdfunding contains heightened information asymmetries The creator must not only produce the product for which they are raising